News Release

Fairness attitudes in children

Psychology: Publication in Communications Psychology

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Heinrich-Heine University Duesseldorf

Experimental setup and schematic diagram of smiley sticker distribution

image: 

Experimental setup and schematic diagram (right side) of smiley sticker distribution. The grey part of each box shows the number of stickers the allocating child awards themselves. The white part shows the number the receiving child is given. In each case, a choice had to be made between a fair 1:1 distribution (each child receives one sticker) or an unfair distribution. The unfair distributions yielded unequal outcomes that were either disadvantageous (left quadrants) or advantageous (right quadrants) for the child making the decision. The distributions were also either non-costly (top quadrants; always one sticker for the child making the decision), or costly (bottom quadrants; the unfair alternative resulted in one more sticker than the fair alternative). (Fig.: HHU/Tobias Kalenscher)

view more 

Credit: HHU/Tobias Kalenscher

How do young children perceive what is fair and what is unfair, and how do they behave as a result? Three psychologists from Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf (HHU), Tilburg University in the Netherlands and the University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, show in the scientific journal Nature Communications Psychology that stereotypical gender differences do exist, but that the story is in fact more complicated than that.

The scenario is familiar: Seven-year-old Lukas complains loudly when his friend Henry is allowed one more scoop of ice cream than him. Although – or even because (?) – he feels unfairly treated, he refuses to share his ice cream with his friend Leo, who has none at all. By contrast, Lisa shares her ice cream with Leo. The next day, however, Lukas has chocolate with him, which he happily shares with Lisa.

The first example seems to fit the stereotype: Boys recognise exactly when they are being disadvantaged, yet at the same time they treat other children just as unfairly. Conversely, girls are more willing to share. But this stereotype does not apply in the case of the chocolate.

Three researchers, who originally all worked at HHU, have examined in more detail how this sense of fairness and unfairness develops in children: Professor Dr Tobias Kalenscher, Principal Investigator of the Comparative Psychology research team in Düsseldorf, Dr Lina Oberließen, now at the Wolf Science Center of the University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, and Professor Dr Marijn van Wingerden from the Department of Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence at Tilburg University. In Nature Communications Psychology, they describe behavioural experiments they have conducted with 332 children aged between three and eight.

Professor van Wingerden: “We did not have ice cream or chocolate though. Instead, the children were paired up and had to award each other smiley stickers. In some cases, we also added additional costs for the allocating child when they e.g. distributed the stickers equally. And then we observed how the children behaved in various gender constellations.”

Dr Oberließen comments on the results: “We indeed found gender-related effects. Girls showed more compassion than boys. Interestingly however, both genders displayed the same envy when a boy received a larger portion. This suggests that boys in general were treated with higher envy.” Boys also tend to be more spiteful to other boys: They always selected the maximum possible number of stickers for themselves, even if it meant that their partner was left empty-handed.

So, the fairness attitudes of children are in fact dependent on gender – however, not only on their own gender, but also on the gender of the children they are interacting with. Van Wingerden: “We identified typical gender stereotypes – girls are more compassionate, while boys are more competitive.” Oberließen adds: “The story is however more complicated than that. Both genders tend to treat boys with more envy than girls. And boys are significantly more compassionate when it comes to sharing resources with girls than with other boys.”

Professor Kalenscher concludes from the results: “Gender stereotypes permeate today’s society. Our study underlines that gendered differences in social behaviour can in fact be observed empirically, even in young children, possibly contributing to cultural gender typecasts in adult life. However, we can also see that, at least in the field of fairness preferences, gendered differences solidify over an extended period. This observation leaves room for promoting non-gender-stereotyped fairness attitudes during this critical period of childhood.”

Original publication:

Marijn van Wingerden, Lina Oberließen & Tobias Kalenscher. Egalitarian preferences in young children depend on the genders of the interacting partners. Communications Psychology 2, 89 (2024).

DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00139-9 


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.