Whether sauntering beside a lake or clambering a mountain, we all adjust our strides to make sure that we’re consuming the least energy possible. ‘People are continuously sensing their energy expenditure and adapting their gait to minimise costs’, says Jessica Selinger from Queen’s University, Canada, adding, ‘We adapt all kinds of gait characteristics – the speed we move at, the length and width of each step – to minimise the calories we are burning’. But it wasn’t clear how our bodies pull off this feat. Are we continually monitoring our manoeuvres and consciously adjusting to ensure that we are moving economically, or are we adapting automatically without realising? And if we are having to think to maintain our efficiency, how much brain power are we having to hold in reserve when multitasking? Intrigued by the possibility that we may have to think harder than we realise to walk efficiently, Rachel Blair (University of British Columbia, Canada), Max Donelan (Simon Fraser University, Canada) and Selinger decided to find out whether people consciously adjust their movements to conserve energy by trying to distract volunteers with a series of beeps while they were walking. The scientists realised that humans do not have to think in order to walk efficiently, they adapt automatically, and Selinger and colleagues publish this discovery in Journal of Experimental Biology at https://journals.biologists.com/jeb.
‘The experimental set-up was technical and had many components’, says Blair, describing how the team started out by getting the volunteers to walk on a treadmill at a comfortable speed as they listened to a stream of beeping tones through headphones, having to press a button in the right hand when the beep was higher than the tone before – or in the left when the beep was lower – to distract them. Then, the team got them to try out different walking styles by moving in step with a metronome while walking at the same speed, setting a faster or slower stride rate, which was either less or more efficient than their normal walk. Once the volunteers had got to grips with walking in different ways, the researchers switched off the metronome, but activated the leg braces that their subjects had been wearing. ‘Many participants assumed that the exoskeleton [leg brace] device was designed to make walking easier’, Selinger says. However, the scientists sometimes used the leg braces to make walking harder – in much the same way that walking upstairs takes more effort – forcing the volunteers to adapt how they walked to find a more efficient style. Finally, the scientists threw everything at the volunteers simultaneously – asking them to walk in different ways while concentrating on recognising whether the tone they were hearing was higher or lower than the previous one, while at the same time trying to find an efficient way of walking in the sometimes-tiring leg braces. ‘It was a challenge to get all these aspects working seamlessly and in-sync with each other’, recalls Blair.
But how efficient would the volunteers be when juggling so many factors? The team reasoned that if they were having to think to find the most efficient way of walking in the leg braces, then the distracting beeping sounds would throw them, and they would use more energy. However, if they were adjusting automatically, then they’d naturally adapt their energy efficiency, despite the beeping distraction.
Patiently analysing the volunteers’ responses, Megan McAllister (Queen’s University) realised that they were able to adjust their walking efficiency automatically, even when distracted by the beeping tones: ‘When people adapt to energy optimal ways of walking, they do so without consciously having to think about it’, she says. And this ability to adapt our movements without thinking allows us to focus on other tasks while we’re walking, including keeping track of bumps in the road and managing our hectic lives.
*********************
IF REPORTING THIS STORY, PLEASE MENTION JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL BIOLOGY AS THE SOURCE AND, IF REPORTING ONLINE, PLEASE CARRY A LINK TO:
or https://journals.biologists.com/jeb/article-lookup/doi/10.1242/jeb.242655
REFERENCE: McAllister, M. J., Blair, R. L., Donelan, J. M. and Selinger, J. C. (2021). Energy optimization during walking involves implicit processing. J. Exp. Biol. 224, jeb242655.
DOI: 10.1242/jeb.242655
This article is posted on this site to give advance access to other authorised media who may wish to report on this story. Full attribution is required, and if reporting online a link to jeb.biologists.com is also required. The story posted here is COPYRIGHTED. Therefore advance permission is required before any and every reproduction of each article in full. PLEASE CONTACT permissions@biologists.com
Journal
Journal of Experimental Biology
Method of Research
Experimental study
Subject of Research
People
Article Title
Energy optimization during walking involves implicit processing
Article Publication Date
14-Sep-2021