News Release

Listeners remember nuances of musical performance, research suggests

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Ohio State University

COLUMBUS, Ohio -- When we hear a piece of music, our memories record not just the melody that makes up its musical structure. We also remember subtle features like intensity and duration of individual notes, say researchers at the Ohio State University. This ability appears as early as 10 months of age, according to a new study by the researchers.

These features are what make two musicians sound different when they are playing the same music, and make two speakers sound different when they are saying the same sentence.

Caroline Palmer, professor of psychology at Ohio State, found that people remember such instance-specific acoustic features - known as prosodic cues - when recalling a familiar tune. Prosodic cues in music enable listeners to identify a favorite performer or a familiar voice in the same way that characteristics of tone and inflection help listeners recognize individual speakers.

Palmer and her collaborators in the research - the late Peter W. Jusczyk, a psychology professor at Johns Hopkins University and Melissa K. Jungers, a graduate student at Ohio State - reported their findings in a recent issue of the Journal of Memory and Language.

The researchers conducted three experiments to investigate people's memory for music. In the first experiment, 24 musically trained subjects and 16 subjects with no musical training were asked to listen to two short musical sequences played on a computer-monitored acoustic piano. The music was played over and over again to familiarize the listeners with the sequences.

The researchers then played different performances of the same musical sequences to listeners. The new performances were exactly the same as the original sequences in melody and rhythm, but they differed in nuances such as how loud and long certain tones were.

Despite the similarity in musical structure, the listeners were able to distinguish the changed sequences from the familiar ones. The subjects who lacked musical training were just as discerning as those who were musically trained.

"The finding showed that listeners were able to remember specific instances in the performance rather than just the general meaning or the "gist" of the music," Palmer said.

To find out if the discerning ability of the subjects was due to their years of exposure to music in everyday life, the researchers repeated the experiment with 16 10-month-old babies. When the babies recognized a piece of music, they turned their heads toward the loudspeakers. The babies turned their heads for longer periods when they heard a performance they had been familiarized with than when they were exposed to novel performances. The researchers believe this indicates that they remembered instance-specific features like the adult subjects.

"We wondered if listeners are equipped from birth with the right perceptual and memory abilities to remember particular musical performances and voices," Palmer said. "The answer was yes. Ten-month-olds can perceive the different performances and remember the ones they have heard before."

From a third experiment, Palmer and her colleagues found that prosodic features that do not match or reinforce the overall musical structure of a composition are the easiest to recognize when presented in the wrong musical context.

"For example, slowing down at the end of a musical phrase is more appropriate than slowing down in the middle of a phrase," Palmer said. When prosody does not fit musical structure, Palmer said, listeners are sensitive to the mismatch between the musical structure and the prosody.

The overall findings of the study show that people remember music in the same way that they remember speech, in which characteristics of tone and inflection distinguish any two speakers. The overlap, Palmer said, could lead to a better understanding of the complexities of human memory.

###

The research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Contact: Caroline Palmer, (614) 292-7718, palmer.1@osu.edu

Written by: Yudhijit Bhattacharjee, (614) 292-8456, bhattacharjee.5@osu.edu


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.