- Individuals with alcohol-induced Korsakoff Syndrome (KS) are often unable to recall recent events.
- A new study investigates if these memory deficits extend to visuoperceptual learning.
- Findings indicate that alcoholics with KS can learn how to perform new tasks without having memory for the items comprising the task.
People with Korsakoff Syndrome (KS), a brain disorder usually associated with long-term heavy drinking and thiamine deficiency, often have profound deficits in their "explicit memory" or ability to recall recent events. A study in the April issue of Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research looks at a memory process called visuoperceptual learning, a component of "implicit memory," which does not require conscious recollection. Results suggest that individuals with KS retain the ability to learn information that is presented visually, even without a conscious recollection of that learning.
"'Explicit events' refer to situations that an individual can consciously recall when asked, 'what did you do yesterday?' or 'what did you do over the holidays?,'" explained Edith Sullivan, a professor in the department of psychiatry and behavioral sciences at Stanford University School of Medicine and corresponding author for the study. "Individuals with KS cannot consciously recall what they did or information presented to them earlier in the day."
"Explicit memory is often referred to as 'knowing what' and implicit memory to 'knowing how,'" said Sara Jo Nixon, professor and director of the Neurocognitive Laboratory at the University of Kentucky. "However, the impact of chronic excessive alcohol consumption on implicit memory has been less clear and less frequently studied. Visuoperceptual tasks likely engage a significant component linked to implicit memory functions. Thus, examining implicit memory using such a task in a comparison of Korsakoff alcoholics, non-Korsakoff alcoholics and controls is an effective way of disentangling alcohol's long-term effects on 'knowing how' versus 'knowing what.' These distinctions are important in considering the process of recovery of function and in identifying brain areas which may be particularly vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol."
Day-to-day examples of visuoperceptual learning, added Sullivan, would be the ability to recognize or recall faces or objects seen previously, or the ability to navigate to a destination after having done so previously.
Sullivan and her colleagues presented the "Gollin Incomplete Pictures Test" to four groups of men: those with KS (n=4), recently detoxified alcoholics without KS (n=9), and two groups of controls, one of age-matched men (n=21) and another of young men (n=6). To examine visuoperceptual learning, the test was given on three occasions, at baseline, one hour later, and one day later.
Both alcoholic groups (KS and the recently detoxified) demonstrated impairments in their visuoperceptual ability. Despite that impairment, all subject groups, even the individuals with KS, showed visuoperceptual learning. In fact, the KS group showed additional learning after continued exposure to the stimuli, despite initial visuospatial deficits and profound explicit memory impairment.
"We found that individuals with KS are able to learn information presented visually, even though they had no conscious recollection of that learning," said Sullivan. "In other words, their behavior shows that learning occurred even though they do not remember having performed the test previously. These findings demonstrate that memory is not a unitary process, but instead consists of a number of different processes and underlying neurological systems. Some, such as conscious declarative memory for newly acquired events, are disrupted in individuals with KS, whereas others, such as visuospatial implicit learning, are spared."
"The findings of the study and their implications are intriguing," added Nixon. "Although learning the task was more difficult for the alcoholics, once learned, their implicit memory processes were relatively unimpaired. The brain areas associated with implicit memory that appear to be relatively spared in chronic alcoholism involve primarily posterior cortical areas."
"This study is consistent with previous studies demonstrating implicit memory processes in KS," said Sullivan, "and expands on previous studies by demonstrating that KS individuals continue to show learning after continued exposure of the information. That is, learning was not only retained after one day, but was enhanced when the subject was presented with the information again."
Nixon added that the study's findings "illustrate that not all types of 'memory' are equally affected and that, indeed, certain fundamental memory functions may be intact even when clinically relevant memory dysfunction exists. These intact functions may provide a foundation for longer-term recovery, at least in non-Korsakoff patients," she said.
Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research (ACER) is the official journal of the Research Society on Alcoholism and the International Society for Biomedical Research on Alcoholism. Co-authors of the ACER paper, "Visuoperceptual Learning in Alcoholic Korsakoff Syndrome" were Rosemary Fama of the Neuroscience Program at SRI International in California; and Adolf Pfefferbaum of the Neuroscience Program at SRI International as well as the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and Neuroscience Program at Stanford University School of Medicine. The study was funded by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.