News Release

Exploring the interactions between baby marmosets and their caregivers

The findings can help understand how parenting impacts the formation of attachment and independence in human children

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Sophia University

Infant-Caregiver Interactions in Marmoset Families

image: 

Marmoset infants adjust their attachments with caregivers and become independent through natural interactions with their families. Now, findings from a new study by researchers from Sophia University provide insights into how parenting shapes the development of independence and attachment in children.

view more 

Credit: Atsuko Saito from Sophia University, Japan

The connection that infants form with their parents or caregivers is crucial for their cognitive, social, and emotional development. These attachments vary in quality, depending on how caregivers respond to the infant's needs. When caregivers are attentive, infants are likely to develop secure attachments. However, if caregivers neglect their needs, infants may develop insecurity, leading to challenges in emotional development and difficulty in forming healthy relationships later in life.

 

To understand how parenting influences attachment formation and child development, researchers led by Associate Professor Atsuko Saito from the Department of Psychology at the Faculty of Human Sciences at Sophia University, and including Saori Yano-Nashimoto (RIKEN Center for Brain Science; Hokkaido University), Anna Truzzi (RIKEN Center for Brain Science; Trinity College Dublin; University of Trento), Kazutaka Shinozuka (RIKEN Center for Brain Science), and Kumi O. Kuroda (RIKEN Center for Brain Science; Tokyo Institute of Technology), studied infant attachment behaviors in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Their findings, published in the journal Communications Biology on February 20, 2024, reveal that infant marmosets adjust their attachment to different caregivers. However, those raised away from their families fail to develop similar adaptive mechanisms and develop attachment disorders.

 

Marmosets are small monkeys native to South America. They have a family structure that has a resemblance to the family structure in humans, where the father, mother, and older siblings take turns carrying the cubs on their backs. The presence of multiple caregivers makes it possible to study the effects of different parenting styles on attachment formation.

 

“Marmoset babies change their attachment behavior depending on their caregivers. Such relationships with families during early childhood are thought to influence the development of attachment and independence, and are expected to provide hints about the development and independence of human children,” explains Dr. Saito.

 

To study the interaction between infants and caregivers, the researchers observed their behavior during reunification. They placed an infant in a basket and the caregiver in an adjacent cage connected by a tunnel with a shutter. On opening the shutter, the researchers observed that infants call out for attention and prefer clinging to the backs of familiar caregivers, such as their parents. When caregivers were patient and attentive, infants stopped calling out once they were picked up. However, the infants avoided intolerant and insensitive caregivers. When picked up by rejecting caregivers, they continued to call out for attention.

 

However, this adaptable attachment behavior was not observed in cubs that were separated from their families in infancy and reared artificially. These cubs avoided their caregivers and tended to stay alone but continued to cry out for care. A significant difference between the two groups was that cubs raised by their families gradually decreased their calls as they grew older. In contrast, artificially reared cubs continued to cry out and seek assistance, even after the second month after birth, indicating a slower development of independence.

 

“These findings show that in marmosets, which have a family structure similar to humans, offspring flexibly change their attachment depending on the family's parenting style, and that the offspring acquires the ability to become independent while being nurtured within the family,” explains Dr. Saito.

 

In most societies, mothers usually take on the primary caregiving role. However, the results suggest that infants can feel equally secure and form strong secure attachments with other supportive caregivers. This indicates that other family members can play an active role in caring for the child, which can ease the responsibilities placed on mothers. Additionally, the study highlights the role of families in fostering independence in infants. Infants become more independent through their interactions with the family.

 

The researchers are currently studying how attachment formation and experiencing attachment disorders affect brain development in marmosets. They aim to use these findings and contribute towards the improvement of parenting practices and childcare in humans.

 

Reference

Title of original paper: 

Anxious about rejection, avoidant of neglect: Infant marmosets tune their attachment based on individual caregiver’s parenting style

Journal:  Communications Biology

DOI:10.1038/s42003-024-05875-6

Authors: 

Saori Yano-Nashimoto1,2, Anna Truzzi1,3,4, Kazutaka Shinozuka1,13, Ayako Y. Murayama1,5,6,14, Takuma Kurachi1,7, Keiko Moriya-Ito8, Hironobu Tokuno8, Eri Miyazawa1, Gianluca Esposito 1,4, Hideyuki Okano5,6, Katsuki Nakamura9, Atsuko Saito1,10, and Kumi O. Kuroda1,11,12

Affiliations:

1Laboratory for Affiliative Social Behavior, RIKEN Center for Brain Science, 2Laboratory of Physiology, Department of Basic Veterinary Sciences, Graduate School of Veterinary Medicine, Hokkaido University, 3Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, 4Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, 5Department of Physiology, Keio University School of Medicine, 6Laboratory for Marmoset Neural Architecture, RIKEN Center for Brain Science, 7Department of Agriculture, Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology, 8Department of Brain & Neurosciences, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, 9Center for the Evolutionary Origins of Human Behavior, Kyoto University, 10Department of Psychology, Sophia University, 11Kuroda Laboratory, School of Life Science and Technology, Tokyo Institute of Technology, 12Laboratory for Circuit and Behavioral Physiology, RIKEN Center for Brain Science, 13Planning, Review and Research Institute for Social insurance and Medical program, 14Neural Circuit Unit, Okinawa Institute Science and Technology Graduate University

 

About Sophia University

Established as a private Jesuit affiliated university in 1913, Sophia University is one of the most prestigious universities located in the heart of Tokyo, Japan. Imparting education through 29 departments in 9 faculties and 25 majors in 10 graduate schools, Sophia hosts more than 13,000 students from around the world.

Conceived with the spirit of “For Others, With Others,” Sophia University truly values internationality and neighborliness, and believes in education and research that go beyond national, linguistic, and academic boundaries. Sophia emphasizes on the need for multidisciplinary and fusion research to find solutions for the most pressing global issues like climate change, poverty, conflict, and violence. Over the course of the last century, Sophia has made dedicated efforts to hone future-ready graduates who can contribute their talents and learnings for the benefit of others, and pave the way for a sustainable future while “Bringing the World Together.”

Website: https://www.sophia.ac.jp/eng/

 

About Associate Professor Atsuko Saito from Sophia University

Atsuko Saito graduated from The University of Tokyo’s Department of Life and Cognitive Sciences, College of Arts and Sciences, and earned her Ph.D. at the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences. She served as an Assistant Professor and Lecturer at The University of Tokyo and subsequently, as a Lecturer at Musashino University’s Faculty of Education, before assuming her current position in 2018.


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.