News Release

How people decide when they have so many choices

Study used eye-trackers to see how people selected snack foods

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Ohio State University

Tracking eyes as they choose a snack

video: This animation shows what one study participant looked at before choosing from among 16 snacks. The current gaze position is indicated by a white box, while the choice is indicated by a red box. For better visibility, gaze durations have been increased by a factor of two. view more 

Credit: Ian Krajbich

COLUMBUS, Ohio - It's one thing to decide among two or three snacks available at a friend's house. But what do people do when they're faced with a vending machine offering 36 different options?

A new study using eye-tracking technology suggests that the amount of time people spend looking at individual items may actually help them decide. Findings showed that people tended to choose snacks they spent more time looking at, sometimes even over snacks that they rated more highly.

"We could do pretty well predicting what people would choose based just on their ratings of the snacks available to them. But we could do an even better job by accounting for how much they looked at each item," said Ian Krajbich, co-author of the study and associate professor of psychology and economics at The Ohio State University.

But the amount of time people spend looking at individual items isn't the whole story of how people decide when they have many alternatives, Krajbich said.

"It's a little more complicated than that," he said.

Krajbich conducted the study with lead author Armin Thomas of Technische Universität Berlin and Felix Molter of Freie Universität Berlin. The research was published this week in the journal eLife.

The study involved 49 people who said they were fans of snack foods and who agreed to fast for at least four hours before the study - to make sure the task was relevant to them.

On a computer screen, the participants were shown sets of 9, 16, 25 or 36 different snack foods and asked to choose which snack they would like to eat the most at the end of the experiment.

They did this multiple times over the course of the experiment. An eye tracker recorded exactly where they looked while making these choices.

After the experiments, participants rated how much they liked all 80 snacks that were part of the study.

The results showed that people didn't look carefully at all the items before making a choice, or even just look at each item until they found one of their favorites. Instead, they looked around in a way that at first glance looked random, but depended on the physical location of the items as well as how much they were liked.

"There is this peripheral screening process where people learn to avoid even looking directly at the snacks they don't really like," Krajbich said.

"This is not something that we see in studies where participants only have two alternatives. It only occurs when they have lots of options."

One of the leading theories among researchers is that when people are presented with many choices, they scan until they find something that is "good enough" - in this case a snack they will enjoy, regardless of whether it is their favorite.

But that's not what happened, Krajbich said. If this "satisficing" model were true, people would quit looking as soon as they found a snack that was good enough. But results showed that participants chose the last snack they looked at only about 45% of the time.

Instead, what seemed to happen most often is that people would look through the items, often going back and forth between them, until one item stood out from the others - often the snack they looked at the most.

"People made a choice when they concluded the best option was sufficiently better than the next-best option," he said.

Where the snacks appeared in the display - left or right, top or bottom - didn't play much of a role in people's decision-making. Participants often started searching in the top left of the display and then looked left to right, top to bottom - but only to a limited extent.

"Pretty quickly their attention gets drawn to their higher-value options. That influences their search process and their gaze starts to jump around less predictably," he said.

###

Contact: Ian Krajbich, Krajbich.1@osu.edu

Written by Jeff Grabmeier, 614-292-8457; Grabmeier.1@osu.edu


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.