News Release

Cultural transmission in bats: When listening for dinner, bats learn from their neighbors

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Cell Press

Fringe-Lipped Bat, <I>Trachops cirrhosus</i>

image: The fringe-lipped bat, Trachops cirrhosus, feeds on frogs and uses frog mating calls to locate and identify its prey. In this photo, a fringe-lipped bat is consuming one of its preferred prey items, the túngara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus. A recent study by Page and Ryan in June 20 Current Biology has shown that bats can rapidly acquire novel foraging responses by observing the behavior of experienced conspecifics. view more 

Credit: This image may only be used to illustrate a news item about this specific researchers work. If you have questions or concerns, please contact Heidi Hardman at 617-397-2879 or <a href="mailto:hhardman@cell.com">hhardman@cell.com</a>

In an exciting study that provides new understanding of how animals learn--and learn from each other--researchers have demonstrated that bats that use frog acoustic cues to find quality prey can rapidly learn these cues by observing other bats. While numerous examples are known of instances where predators can use so-called "social learning" to learn new visual and olfactory cues associated with prey, this kind of learning of an acoustic cue had not been previously described. The work is reported by Rachel A. Page and Michael J. Ryan of the University of Texas at Austin and the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute and appears in the June 20th issue of Current Biology.

The fringe-lipped bat, Trachops cirrhosus, uses frog calls from different species as acoustic cues to assess the palatability of its prey. Previous experiments have shown that T. cirrhosus is extremely flexible in its foraging behavior. In the new study, Page and Ryan investigated the role of social learning in bat foraging flexibility. Comparing three different learning groups, the researchers measured the rate at which bats learned new foraging information: in this case, the novel (experimental) association of the calls of a poisonous toad species with the presence of palatable prey. The researchers tested the effectiveness of learning this experimental association through three different means: (a) a social learning group, in which a bat inexperienced with the new call-food association was allowed to observe an experienced bat; (b) a social facilitation group, in which two inexperienced bats were presented with the experimental task together; and (c) a trial-and-error group, in which a single inexperienced bat was presented with the experimental task alone. In the social learning group, bats rapidly acquired the novel association in an average of 5.3 trials. In the social facilitation and trial-and-error groups, most bats did not approach the call of the poisonous species even after 100 trials. These results suggest that once acquired, novel prey-cue/prey-quality associations could spread rapidly through bat populations by cultural transmission.

###

The researchers include Rachel A. Page and Michael J. Ryan of the University of Texas at Austin in Austin, TX and the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Balboa, Panama.

Funding was provided to R.A.P by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship and the Theodore Roosevelt Memorial Fund of the American Museum of Natural History.

Page et al.: "Social Transmission of Novel Foraging Behavior in Bats: Frog Calls and Their Referents." Publishing in Current Biology 16, 1201–1205, June 20, 2006, DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.038. www.current-biology.com

Related Dispatch by Ratcliffe et al.: "Animal Behavior: Who Will Croak Next?"


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.